首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   220篇
  免费   2篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   183篇
科学研究   5篇
各国文化   2篇
体育   11篇
文化理论   2篇
信息传播   20篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   9篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   54篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   8篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   6篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   7篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1964年   1篇
排序方式: 共有223条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
The debate on whether kinesiology students should undergo fitness testing is controversial. Some are strong proponents of fitness testing, while others are in strong opposition. As advocates for fitness testing among kinesiology majors, we aimed to assess the fitness levels of Exercise Science and Physical Education Teacher Education majors and evaluate current physical education teacher education (PETE) fitness criterion. One hundred and sixteen participants (males = 49; females = 67) underwent health-related fitness assessments. 37% of males and 51% of females met PETE’s fitness criteria. Both groups performed significantly better in body fat percentage and upper body strength than PETE’s acceptable (average) standards; however, most were significantly lower than target (above average) standards. Presented are fitness passing criteria for PETE majors and fitness reporting that aids in improved fitness tracking and accountability. We also highlight the need for NASPE and SHAPE America to clearly define fitness expectations for PETE majors and encourage other kinesiology faculty to consider implementing fitness testing in their programs.  相似文献   
73.
A report of an experiment to measure the effects of both overt and covert responses to educational stimuli with KCR (knowledge of correct results) feedback provided. In most studies of feedback, KCR (or KR) is given as a consequence of an overt response: Stimulus-Response-Feedback. In the case of a covert response, the student constructs his answer but does not react overtly to the presentation of a stimulus item. Forty Ss were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 groups further divided into 1 control group receiving no feedback and 3 groups receiving information (KCR) according to either overt or covert response patterns. A 25-item multiple-choice test was administered 5 times by means of a terminal consisting of a screen and a teletype. Results showed that the covert paradigm was the most effective. The experiment did not demonstrate that the covert process leads to better performance on a test administered later to measure retention. A tentative explanation for this negative result was that the time interval between the provision of feedback and presentation of the next item was of some relevance. A multiple comparison test revealed that the 3 treatment groups performed better than the control group.  相似文献   
74.
Over 500 men who attended the Gow School, an independent school for boys with developmental dyslexia, were given a follow-up questionnaire from 1 to 38 years after they left the school. More than half had graduated from college; business was the most frequently chosen major in college; most were employed in managerial or related business positions; and their adult reading habits and attitudes did not compare well with those of other men of similar socioeconomic backgrounds. Socioeconomic status and IQ were not predictive of adult outcome among these men in contrast to their effect in the general population. An important result was that severity of the reading problem upon entrance to Gow and the academic and remedial progress of the men while at the school were highly predictive of adult educational, occupational, and attitudinal status. This work was supported by funds from NIH grant HD00486, the Gow School, and the John F. Kennedy Institute.  相似文献   
75.
76.
Preschool is an institution for cultural preservation, which responds to social change. Guided by this assumption, this study used a recent video of Chinese preschool in conjunction with the questionnaire from Preschool in Three Cultures, a study by Joseph Tobin and his colleagues in the 1980s, to examine the cultural and professional beliefs of three groups of U.S. early childhood educators (preservice, inservice non-Head Start, and inservice Head Start). We examined participants’ cultural beliefs, critical comments on the preschool, and group differences in their comments. Results show that participants’ beliefs about necessary qualities for a teacher remained stable from the 1980s in the U.S. But a shift occurred in beliefs about why society needs preschools. More education and longer employment were linked significantly to more critical comments of Chinese practice. There was a marked difference in professional and cultural beliefs by educators’ group affiliations.
Yeh HsuehEmail:
  相似文献   
77.
78.
The paper evaluates the work of the International Adult Literacy Survey as reported in OECD 1997. It assesses its contribution to understanding literacy in terms of the perspective of the New Literacy Studies. It outlines this perspective as a basis for a critique that is mostly concerned with the validity of the test. Three criticisms of the survey are made: that it provides only a partial picture of literacy; that culture is treated as bias; and that the test items do not represent the real-life items as claimed. Finally, the paper concludes with an overall evaluation of what the IALS achieves in terms of its own aims.  相似文献   
79.
The stressors associated with poverty increase the risks for externalizing psychopathology; however, specific patterns of neurobiology and higher self-regulation may buffer against these effects. This study leveraged a randomized control trial, aimed at increasing self-regulation at ~11 years of age. As adults, these same individuals completed functional MRI scanning (Mage = 24.88 years; intervention n = 44; control n = 49). Functional connectivity between the hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex was examined in relation to the intervention, gains in self-regulation, and present-day externalizing symptoms. Increased connectivity between these brain areas was noted in the intervention group compared to controls. Furthermore, individual gains in self-regulation, instilled by the intervention, statistically explained this brain difference. These results begin to connect neurobiological and psychosocial markers of risk and resiliency.  相似文献   
80.
In collaborative decision-making, partners compare reasons behind conflicting proposals through meta-talk. We investigated UK-based preschoolers’ (mixed socioeconomic status) use of meta-talk (Data collection: 2018–2020). In Study 1, 5- and 7-year-old peer dyads (N = 128, 61 girls) heard conflicting claims about an animal from two informants. One prefaced her claim with “I know”; the other with “I think”. Dyads identified the more reliable informant through meta-talk (“She said she knows”). In Study 2, 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 64, 34 girls) searched for a toy with an adult partner making incorrect proposals. Children refuted this through reporting what they had witnessed (It cannot be there because “I saw it move”, “she moved it”). In preschool period, children start using meta-talk to make rational collaborative decisions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号