首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   9989篇
  免费   122篇
  国内免费   8篇
教育   7241篇
科学研究   947篇
各国文化   111篇
体育   630篇
综合类   11篇
文化理论   179篇
信息传播   1000篇
  2022年   36篇
  2021年   90篇
  2020年   173篇
  2019年   231篇
  2018年   303篇
  2017年   395篇
  2016年   434篇
  2015年   284篇
  2014年   265篇
  2013年   1607篇
  2012年   356篇
  2011年   382篇
  2010年   261篇
  2009年   205篇
  2008年   282篇
  2007年   251篇
  2006年   218篇
  2005年   838篇
  2004年   618篇
  2003年   456篇
  2002年   273篇
  2001年   199篇
  2000年   158篇
  1999年   112篇
  1998年   82篇
  1997年   101篇
  1996年   97篇
  1995年   73篇
  1994年   87篇
  1993年   76篇
  1992年   91篇
  1991年   73篇
  1990年   73篇
  1989年   54篇
  1988年   52篇
  1987年   56篇
  1986年   73篇
  1985年   68篇
  1984年   61篇
  1983年   63篇
  1982年   50篇
  1981年   36篇
  1980年   42篇
  1979年   51篇
  1978年   48篇
  1977年   30篇
  1976年   30篇
  1975年   22篇
  1974年   17篇
  1948年   18篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
64.
ABSTRACT

The literature on momentum is still undecided, with mixed results whether momentum exists or is only perceived to exist (“hot hand fallacy”). We explore whether momentum exists by looking at cases in which a basketball player has three consecutive free throws. A free throw is a well-defined task executed in a stable environment, allegedly giving momentum optimal chances to occur. Taking 14 NBA seasons we collected over 4500 three-free-throw sets (triplets). We obtained the outcomes of the shots as well as some additional variables about the player and the game: the player’s average free-throw percentage, home or away, and the game score and the quarter when the free throws were attempted. We first analyse the hit rates in the three shots and then proceed to regression analysis that also controls for the abovementioned variables. We address several concerns raised in the literature. All comparisons and analyses yield the same conclusion that there is no evidence for momentum in the data.  相似文献   
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
In many Western science systems, funding structures increasingly stimulate academic research to contribute to practical applications, but at the same time the rise of bibliometric performance assessments have strengthened the pressure on academics to conduct excellent basic research that can be published in scholarly literature. We analyze the interplay between these two developments in a set of three case studies of fields of chemistry in the Netherlands. First, we describe how the conditions under which academic chemists work have changed since 1975. Second, we investigate whether practical applications have become a source of credibility for individual researchers. Indeed, this turns out to be the case in catalysis, where connecting with industrial applications helps in many steps of the credibility cycle. Practical applications yield much less credibility in environmental chemistry, where application-oriented research agendas help to acquire funding, but not to publish prestigious papers or to earn peer recognition. In biochemistry practical applications hardly help in gaining credibility, as this field is still strongly oriented at fundamental questions. The differences between the fields can be explained by the presence or absence of powerful upstream end-users, who can afford to invest in academic research with promising long term benefits.  相似文献   
70.
In collaborative problem solving, children produce and evaluate arguments for proposals. We investigated whether 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 192) can produce and evaluate arguments against those arguments (i.e., counter-arguments). In Study 1, each child within a peer dyad was privately given a reason to prefer one over another solution to a task. One child, however, was given further information that would refute the reasoning of their partner. Five-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, identified and produced valid and relevant counter-arguments. In Study 2, 3-year-olds were given discourse training (discourse that contrasted valid and invalid counter-arguments) and then given the same problem-solving tasks. After training, 3-year-olds could also identify and produce valid and relevant counter-arguments. Thus, participating in discourse about reasons facilitates children’s counter-argumentation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号