首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   499篇
  免费   5篇
教育   385篇
科学研究   19篇
各国文化   4篇
体育   42篇
文化理论   4篇
信息传播   50篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   17篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   22篇
  2015年   20篇
  2014年   16篇
  2013年   106篇
  2012年   13篇
  2011年   12篇
  2010年   6篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   11篇
  2005年   15篇
  2004年   13篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   7篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   4篇
  1982年   9篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   4篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   3篇
  1974年   4篇
  1972年   3篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   3篇
  1942年   2篇
  1940年   2篇
  1835年   2篇
排序方式: 共有504条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
101.
102.
Theories of self-regulated learning assume that learners flexibly adapt their learning process to external task demands and that this is positively related to performance. In this study, university students (n = 119) solved three tasks that greatly differed in complexity. Their learning processes were captured in detail by task-specific questionnaires and computer-generated log files. Results indicate that students adapted almost all learning processes significantly to task complexity. For example, students accessed more hypertext pages for complex tasks than for simple tasks. However, this kind of adaptation was not consistently related to performance. For variables capturing learners’ self-regulation, such as the number of accessed hypertext pages, more pronounced adaptation was significantly and positively related to performance even when learners’ general processing depth was statistically controlled. Results were less consistent for variables capturing learners’ self-monitoring, such as their judged task complexity.  相似文献   
103.
104.
105.
106.
According to Wegner's (1994) theory of ironic processes of mental control, self-instructions not to perform in a certain way, together with mental loads, can induce thoughts, emotions, and behaviours that are precisely the opposite of intention. Wegner's theory was tested against the implicit overcompensation hypothesis, which states that movement direction and magnitude are implicitly dictated by self-instruction, irrespective of load, promoting overcompensation of action. Two experiments were conducted using a golf-putting task. In Experiment 1, 48 participants were randomly assigned to one of four load conditions: cognitive, visual, auditory, and self-presentation/incentive. In the experimental trials, participants were instructed to make the putt, but it was emphasized that the putt should not be left short of the target. Following the instructional strategy, putts landed significantly longer than at baseline, irrespective of load. In Experiment 2, 36 participants were divided into one of three groups, without load, in which different instructional sets (control, not putting long, and not putting short) were emphasized. A significant interaction emerged, as participants putted significantly longer or shorter than controls depending on the instructional set given. Overall, our findings support the implicit overcompensation hypothesis. Theoretical and practical considerations are discussed.  相似文献   
107.
108.
Significant gains in conservation of number, amount, mass, and weight were made by 53 nonconservers (age 6-9) who either simply observed conserver's performance on mass and weight problems or argued with conservers in a social conflict situation about these problems. There were no significant differences in conservation performance between the modeling and social conflict treatments. It was suggested that the social conflict effect could be attributed to modeling effects.  相似文献   
109.
110.
This paper uses case studies to focus on distance learning in developing countries as an enabler for economic development and poverty reduction. To provide perspective, we first review the history of telecottages, local technology-equipped facilities to foster community-based learning, which have evolved into “telecenters” or “Community Learning Centers” (CLCs). Second, we describe extensive site visits to CLCs in impoverished portions of China and Mexico, the centers operated by premier universities in each respective country. These CLCs constitute the core of new emerging systems of distance education, and their newness poses challenges and opportunities, which are discussed. Finally, we offer 12 points to develop further the concept and reality of distance learning in support of economic development.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号