首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   933篇
  免费   9篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   695篇
科学研究   47篇
各国文化   6篇
体育   56篇
文化理论   8篇
信息传播   131篇
  2023年   4篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   22篇
  2019年   19篇
  2018年   35篇
  2017年   38篇
  2016年   29篇
  2015年   24篇
  2014年   26篇
  2013年   252篇
  2012年   17篇
  2011年   32篇
  2010年   42篇
  2009年   37篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   14篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   10篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   9篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   13篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   17篇
  1998年   10篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   4篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   6篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   7篇
  1974年   5篇
  1969年   3篇
  1968年   6篇
  1967年   6篇
  1966年   5篇
排序方式: 共有943条查询结果,搜索用时 359 毫秒
111.
Conclusion The purpose of this paper is not to denigrate pencil-and-paper tests of intellectual capabilities. The Understanding in Science Test, if it were not for its reliability problems, may be a useful test of intellectual processing. It appears not to be a successful analogue to clinically-administered Piagetian tasks. The same may be said of the other written or group tests of this nature. If they are reliable, they may provide useful measures of intellectual ability. Our research, though, suggests that, for junior high school students and younger, they may not possess high concurrent validity with Piagetian tasks.  相似文献   
112.
The language children hear presents them with a multitude of co-occurrences between words and things in the world, and they must repeatedly determine which among these manifold co-occurrences is relevant. Social factors—such as cues regarding the speaker's referential intent—might serve as one guide to whether word-object covariation should be registered. In 2 studies, infants (15–20 months and 18–20 months in Studies 1 and 2, respectively) heard novel labels at a time when they were investigating a single novel object; in one case the label was uttered by a speaker seated within the infant's view and displaying concurrent attention to the novel toy (coupled condition), whereas in the other case the label emanated from a speaker seated out of the infant's view (decoupled condition). In both studies, subsequent comprehension questions indicated that infants of 18–20 months registered a stable link between label and object in the coupled condition, but not in the decoupled condition, despite the fact that covariation between label and object was equivalent in the 2 conditions. Thus, by 18–20 months children are inclined to establish a mapping between word and object only when a speaker displays signs of referring to that object.  相似文献   
113.
在选择考研的学校时,考哪个学校是和考哪个专业一样重要的问题,考名校还是考一般的学校,考本校还是外校,这是个问题.我的观点是,还是考名校比较好.  相似文献   
114.
115.
116.
117.
Gender in plants     
Complex evolutionary forces determine whether plants are male, female or hermaphrodite throughout their lives or whether they switch from one sex to another. This article explores these often conflicting forces and seeks answers for sex change in plants.  相似文献   
118.
Gender in plants     
What is the difference between plant sex and plant gender? Why does stress sometimes cause maleness and other times result in femaleness? Do plant hedge mating bets using sex change? This article addresses these and other evolutionary questions about plant gender.  相似文献   
119.
The genetic and environmental contributions to children's maladaptive behavior are assessed in a sample of 154 twin pairs (77 MZ twin pairs and 77 DZ twin pairs), who range in age from 6 to 11 years. To bridge the strengths of behavioral genetic methods and environmental assessment techniques, we use a multimethod, multimeasure approach to data collection, and analyze the data using behavioral genetic modeling techniques. Results indicate that genetic variation accounts for a majority of the variance in parent-reported child maladaptive behavior (average = 62%). One parent-report measure also suggests a smaller, significant contribution of shared environmental variance. In contrast to the parental ratings, the observational coding and global impressions of parent-twin interactive behavior suggest that shared environment is the primary source of variance accounting for parent and child maladaptive behavior. This is due, in part, to the direct influence one's interactive partner has on the expression of maladaptive behavior in an interactive setting. When controlling for the co-participant's behavior, genetic variation increases and shared environmental variation decreases.  相似文献   
120.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号