首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   26636篇
  免费   385篇
  国内免费   31篇
教育   18330篇
科学研究   2699篇
各国文化   285篇
体育   2533篇
综合类   28篇
文化理论   204篇
信息传播   2973篇
  2021年   236篇
  2020年   381篇
  2019年   535篇
  2018年   691篇
  2017年   739篇
  2016年   699篇
  2015年   457篇
  2014年   607篇
  2013年   4823篇
  2012年   550篇
  2011年   605篇
  2010年   508篇
  2009年   483篇
  2008年   526篇
  2007年   484篇
  2006年   465篇
  2005年   426篇
  2004年   691篇
  2003年   550篇
  2002年   541篇
  2001年   668篇
  2000年   522篇
  1999年   440篇
  1998年   216篇
  1997年   270篇
  1996年   279篇
  1995年   256篇
  1994年   269篇
  1993年   224篇
  1992年   349篇
  1991年   350篇
  1990年   343篇
  1989年   392篇
  1988年   331篇
  1987年   353篇
  1986年   337篇
  1985年   419篇
  1984年   335篇
  1983年   311篇
  1982年   273篇
  1981年   213篇
  1980年   192篇
  1979年   299篇
  1978年   234篇
  1977年   207篇
  1976年   194篇
  1975年   187篇
  1974年   199篇
  1973年   173篇
  1971年   174篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
121.
122.
对保龄球运动员而言,影响其训练与竞赛的心理因素包括:运动表象、运动情绪和运动员的注意特征等。对保龄球运动员的心理因素进行了分析,旨在探讨运动员心理因素与运动技能完成过程之间的关系,提出控制、调整运动员心理因素的对策。  相似文献   
123.
124.
125.
126.
127.
对我国运动员进行心理表象训练新程序的理论探讨   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
表象训练是体育运动实践中运用最广泛的训练方法之一。我国在此领域远远落后于日本,其主要因素包括表象训练的方法、个体差异和情境因素:日本新近开发的心理表象训练程序(简称MITP),几经实践运用,不仅训练效果良好而且在运动训练和比赛心理的理论方面产生了积极的影响,检验了有关表象训练的理论问题,认为动觉表象、视觉表象和想象表象有机结合是提高心理训练效果的良好途径;对MITP作一些理论探讨,目的在于为我国体育运动心理学理论建设提供一点参考。  相似文献   
128.
This paper considers the background of the Library-Bibliographic Classification as the national classification system of Russia, and its history and application in the USSR. It gives the general characteristic of the LBC structure, content and system of designations, as well as the state of its present updating.  相似文献   
129.
Between 1880 and 1950, the role of women as authors of elementary readers flowered and faded. From 1880 to 1915, female authorship dominated. Over the next fifteen years female authorship withstood the challenge of the scientific movement in education. Women’s role as solo authors declined, however, during the Depression, World War II and the postwar years. These changes are explained by the deteriorating environment for female professionalism, changes in the concept of reading expertise, the expanded role of publishers in textbook development, and the differences between men and women in opportunities for graduate study. E. Jennifer is Monaghan a professor in the Department of Educational Services, Brooklyn College of the City University of New York. She is the founder of the History of Reading Special Interest Group of the International Reading Association and author ofA Common Heritage: Noah Webster’s Blue-Back Speller.  相似文献   
130.
The aim was to examine cross-cultural variation in linguistic responsiveness to young children in 10 English-speaking mother–child dyads and 10 Italian-speaking mother– child dyads. All 20 children were late talkers who possessed delays in expressive vocabulary development but age-appropriate cognitive and receptive language skills. Dyads were filmed in 15 minute free play contexts, which were transcribed and coded for measures of maternal linguistic input (e.g. rate, MLU, labels, expansions) and child language productivity (e.g. utterances, different words used). The results revealed that the Italian mothers used more utterances, spoke more quickly and used a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian mothers. The Italian children mirrored their mothers and also used more utterances and a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian children. Mothers in both groups used similar percentages of responsive labels and expansions. However, Italian mothers responded to fewer of their children's vocalisations, using a smaller percentage of imitations and interpretations than the Canadian mothers. Correlations between maternal input and children's language productivity revealed that contingent language measures (e.g. imitations, interpretations, expansions) were related to high levels of productivity in children in both cultural groups. The results support the use of language interventions based on increasing maternal responsiveness for these children at the one-word stage of language development. They also point to differences that may be culturally based. For example, Italian mothers use faster rates of interaction and appear to have higher expectations for their children's verbal participation in interaction. This is reflected in higher rates of language production from their children, even though children in both cultural groups have similar vocabulary sizes.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号