首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   209篇
  免费   0篇
  国内免费   1篇
教育   145篇
科学研究   13篇
各国文化   1篇
体育   32篇
文化理论   5篇
信息传播   14篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   6篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   3篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   30篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   6篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   4篇
  2006年   7篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   2篇
  1997年   4篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   4篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   1篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   2篇
  1965年   1篇
  1964年   2篇
  1963年   1篇
  1962年   1篇
  1959年   2篇
  1957年   3篇
  1924年   2篇
排序方式: 共有210条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study is to examine processing of facial emotions in a sample of maltreated children showing high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Maltreatment during childhood has been associated independently with both atypical processing of emotion and the development of PTSD. However, research has provided little evidence indicating how high rates of PTSD might relate to maltreated children's processing of emotions. METHOD: Participants' reaction time and labeling of emotions were measured using a morphed facial emotion identification task. Participants included a diverse sample of maltreated children with and without PTSD and controls ranging in age from 8 to 15 years. Maltreated children had been removed from their homes and placed in state custody following experiences of maltreatment. Diagnoses of PTSD and other disorders were determined through combination of parent, child, and teacher reports. RESULTS: Maltreated children displayed faster reaction times than controls when labeling emotional facial expressions, and this result was most pronounced for fearful faces. Relative to children who were not maltreated, maltreated children both with and without PTSD showed enhanced response times when identifying fearful faces. There was no group difference in labeling of emotions when identifying different facial emotions. CONCLUSIONS: Maltreated children show heightened ability to identify fearful faces, evidenced by faster reaction times relative to controls. This association between maltreatment and atypical processing of emotion is independent of PTSD diagnosis.  相似文献   
4.
Previous investigations have shown that teachers have a clear idea of the typical characteristics of gifted pupils. The present study investigates whether these features are combined linearly or whether they are used to activate pupil exemplars or prototypes stored in the longterm memory, when teachers are set the task of distinguishing gifted from above average pupils. A group of 37 teachers at grammar schools in the German federal state of Baden‐Württemberg, who had all had adequate experience with gifted children, answered written questions. Their judgement behaviour was modelled with the help of discriminant analyses in which a number of different predictor sets were included. These analyses revealed that both individual features and prototypical patterns of features seem to be used simultaneously. In contrast, personal experience with individual highly gifted pupils seemed not to play any role, although the method used could be expected to conceal the effects of personal experience.  相似文献   
5.
6.

Two levels of training (100 vs. 500 trials) and two ages of rats (young and adult) were used in a developmental analysis of the relationship between response strength and the effects of punishment. The apparatus was a Y maze with three discriminably different arms. After 100 or 500 reinforced trials, subjects were shocked each time they responded in one arm. The recovery sessions followed the punishment session. Results from the punishment day indicated that: (a) young rats received a greater amount of shock, and (b) additional training increased the amount of shock received by the young but decreased it in the adults. The recovery data showed that: (a) the suppressive effects of punishment were greater for the adults than for the young, and (b) the recovery scores were not influenced by degree of overtraining. The Age by Overtraining interaction suggested that the relationship between response strength and punishment is age dependent. The age differences found with the amount-of-shock and recovery measures provided additional support for the position that younger rats are less competent than adult rats in inhibiting responses.

  相似文献   
7.
Abstract

In this editorial, we provide an introduction to the special issue on Inquiry-Based Learning in First and Second Year Courses. We also discuss the essential features of inquiry-based learning and provide a brief overview of the literature and evidence for its effectiveness.  相似文献   
8.
In this article we address "language approach" data as a key variable in quantitative, large-scale research on educational achievement, focusing on our work for the Achievements of Deaf Pupils in Scotland (ADPS) project. The complexity of approaches is addressed, with a particular focus on a "no-exclusion" model of service. In this context 3 years of language-related data are discussed, using constructions of language variables that take into account the variability in deaf pupils' hearing loss levels, types of provision, and professional practice. We see this as a necessary first step toward offering a nuanced context for understanding patterns in the educational outcomes among the ADPS population to be reported in a later article. The ADPS data on language approach can reveal general patterns at macro levels: our analysis suggests that, in Scotland, the extent and quality of British Sign Language/English provision may be determined more by local factors than by linguistic requirements and that ostensibly responsive policies can mask a limited spectrum for pupils and their families. However, the ADPS data are insufficiently sensitive to detailed and local variations to reflect the full complexity of language situations over time-a situation which represents an ongoing challenge for all long-term, large-scale studies.  相似文献   
9.
制度理论一直将教育领域作为一个重要的研究对象。近十几年来,在教育领域出现了三个显著的变化,它们分别是教育服务提供从单一走向多元、教育系统结构从松散变为紧密以及教育制度重要性的增加。这些教育制度变化的新对于以往制度理论的基本命题提出了挑战,要求重新审视一些制度关系,如同形性与多样性、合法性与效率、松散性与紧密性、制度约束与制度创新、政府与市场等。这预示着对于教育领域进行制度研究的广阔前景。  相似文献   
10.
The increased use of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) medication by children has led to growing concerns. In a previous study, we found that many of the teachers that were interviewed about ADHD spontaneously associated ADHD with medication. The present study is a qualitative reanalysis of what precisely these 30 primary school teachers had spontaneously said about medication in our previous semi‐structured interviews on ADHD. Almost all respondents had experience with pupils taking ADHD medication. The majority of spontaneously mentions medication as the treatment of ADHD. Attitudes towards ADHD medication use by pupils are mainly ambivalent, but more positive than negative effects of medication are reported. However, what teachers say about ADHD medication is often not based on sound information; their attitudes tend to be formed by personal experiences rather than founded on professional and scientific sources. We conclude from our analysis that it will be in the interest of reducing the number of children on ADHD medication that teachers have good access to verified and up‐to‐date information on ADHD and medication so that they are better supported in making evidence‐based pedagogical judgments.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号