首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   227篇
  免费   4篇
教育   151篇
科学研究   33篇
各国文化   3篇
体育   20篇
文化理论   2篇
信息传播   22篇
  2023年   3篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   17篇
  2018年   6篇
  2017年   10篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   4篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   8篇
  2007年   5篇
  2006年   7篇
  2005年   5篇
  2004年   4篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   3篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   2篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1982年   3篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   2篇
  1951年   6篇
  1946年   3篇
  1941年   1篇
  1939年   1篇
  1937年   1篇
  1935年   1篇
  1927年   1篇
  1925年   2篇
  1922年   1篇
  1892年   1篇
  1859年   1篇
  1840年   1篇
  1828年   1篇
排序方式: 共有231条查询结果,搜索用时 250 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
Word-learning skills of 19 deaf/hard-of-hearing preschoolers were assessed by observing their ability to learn new words in two contexts. The first context required the use of a novel mapping strategy (i.e., making the inference that a novel word refers to a novel object) to learn the new words. The second context assessed the ability to learn new words after minimal exposure when reference was explicitly established. The children displayed three levels of word-learning skills. Eleven children learned words in both contexts. Five were able to learn new words rapidly only when reference was explicitly established. Two children did not learn new words rapidly in either context. The latter seven children were followed longitudinally. All children eventually acquired the ability to learn new words in both contexts. The deaf children's word-learning abilities were related to the size of their vocabularies. The present study suggests that word-learning strategies are acquired even when children are severely delayed in their language development and they learn language in an atypical environment.  相似文献   
4.
Educational technology research and development - Authentic project-based learning (APBL) is a highly effective way for instructors to help students learn disciplinary skills, modes of thinking,...  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
Executive functions enable flexible thinking, something young children are notoriously bad at. For instance, in the dimensional change card sort (DCCS) task, 3‐year‐olds can sort cards by one dimension (shape), but continue to sort by this dimension when asked to switch (to color). This study tests a prediction of a dynamic neural field model that prior experience with the postswitch dimension can enhance 3‐year‐olds' performance in the DCCS. In Experiment 1A, a matching game was used to preexpose 3‐year‐olds (= 36) to color. This facilitated switching from sorting by shape to color. In 3 , 3‐year‐olds (n = 18) were preexposed to shape. This did not facilitate switching from sorting by color to shape. The model was used to explain this asymmetry.  相似文献   
8.
The remit for this short piece was to identify themes emerging from the articles in this edition by Julie McLeod, Duncan Waite and Eugenie Samier, to consider how these themes reflect on the current field, and to identify their ongoing relevance. Additionally, I was asked to consider the current challenges that the field is facing and what this means for research and journals such as this one. My short response is that our research has to matter. It has to matter to us as individuals so that what we do is worthwhile; it has to matter that we can provide rigorous research that is reliable so that it can inform policy makers and administrators; and it has to provide teachers and students with a greater understanding of why they are doing what they are being asked to do.  相似文献   
9.
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号